Chances are if you are reading this you may be a musician, music educator or music student. If you are such a person, you may have heard non-musicians refer to you as ‘talented’. Being referred to as talented implies you may have born with a special ability that those that aren’t as musical you, don’t possess. Have you ever wondered if this is true? At times, musicians do seem different than non-musicians. Are these differences innate or does our environment shape our musical future? The age-old question of nature vs. nurture crops up yet again. Fortunately we now possess tools and have developed skills to help answer these questions. The development of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) and improvements in image resolution due to technology advances and increased computer power has provided us with the means to peer into the brain non-invasively (Laufs, 2012).
[accordion multiopen=”true”]
[toggle title=”A Little on Brain Structure”]
Reading about neuroscience can be daunting partially due to unfamiliar terminology. If you get overwhelmed, this site http://www.g2conline.org/ is a very helpful resource. It includes a 3D model and peer reviewed research reviews on brain structure. The structures of the brain are organized into headings and subheadings, and it includes a search bar and video links. I have found it invaluable and refer to it regularly to clarify brain structure and function, as well as to explore articles on related topics.
I have assembled a playlist with some useful links to help clarify the structure of the brain, its nomenclature and some things about its function. This 5 part video series entitled “Brain Matters” is especially clear and helpful:
https://www.youtube.com/playlist?list=PLgkIZ87-dg0VWRf-8ur1aJvpdGkaw7qvn
Question: Can you suggest other sources for brain structure and function?
Brain development can be compared to muscle development; the parts that are used the most get stronger. Musical activities are a great workout for your brain and often for your body too.
Grey Matter and White Matter
A few simplified definitions may be helpful when discussing brain function.
Grey Matter
Neurons- A category of specialized cells that handle information going to and from and within the brain.
Dendrites- The portion of the brain cell responsible for collecting incoming signals.
Axon- The portion of the brain cell responsible for carrying signals away from the cell body.
White Matter
Myelin- Allows faster signal rate transfer by the axon.
We can think of the structural portions of the neuron as grey matter and the myelin around the axon as white matter. There are many types of neurons and myelinated cells due to the variety of functions they are required to handle.
Basic Neuron Function
Neurons can communicate within the neuron and between neurons. Dendrites accept information from other neurons and house it in the cell body until the signal is strong enough for the cell to send a signal to other neurons down the axon. The combined signal collected by the dendrite must strong enough to be transferred to the next neuron; this is called action potential. If the dendrites have collected enough signal for the cell body to send the signal through the axon it has reached its ‘action potential’. If the dendrites have not provided enough external signal, no signal is passed along the axon. The axon terminals connect to the dendrites of other cells through neurotransmitters; an area called the synapse.
Synapse- The area of communication between brain cells.
[/toggle]
[toggle title=”Nature Vs. Nurture, Are Musicians Born with Musical Ability?”]
The short answer to this question is – maybe. fMRI evidence points to differences in development in various brain areas of musicians due to practice and exposure to music, this phenomenon is referred to as ‘brain plasticity’ (Berlucchi & Buchtel, 2009). A multi-tiered, 2 year longitudinal study was designed to determine if individuals were potentially predisposed to musicality. In the first tier, a group of 5-7 year old children with no prior musical training were given extra instrumental lessons and compared with a control group who had no prior training and did not receive music lessons either. fMRI results were combined with a battery of other cognitive tests to establish a baseline measurement. No significant structural differences in grey matter, white matter or corpus callosum size (the tissue of the brain responsible for connecting the left and right hemispheres) were detected in either group at baseline. However, after one year of training a significant increase in grey and white matter in these areas was found in the test group.
The second tier of the study was a cross-sectional comparison of children between the ages of 9-11; each possessing an average of 3-4 years of instrumental music instruction. Once more, a significantly larger volume of grey and white matter was found in the instrumental group than in the control group (Schlaug, 2006).
A previous cross-sectional study was used as the third tier of the study, using adults as participants. The study used three categories for comparison; non-musicians, amateur musicians, and professional musicians. And once again, significantly more grey and white matter was found in the brain structures of professional musicians than compared the other two groups. (Gaser & Schlaug, 2003; Schlaug, 2006). These findings indicate that increased grey matter development was due to the amount of musical training, practicing and performing one engages in.
[/toggle]
[toggle title=”Maybe Some People are Wired for Music?”]
There is a distinction between being born with musical ability and being genetically predisposed with the potential to focus on musical activities. The former implies that those who are born with this ability don’t have to work as hard to achieve musical success, the later implies certain people may possess the hereditary potential to succeed at music.
15 children with a mean age of 6.32 years old were given 15 months of private keyboard lessons (i.e. outside of the school system) weekly for 30 minutes. This group was compared with a control group of 16 children of a mean age of 5.90 years old who did not receive private lessons but who did participate in a weekly 40 minute group music class in school. The results of the experiment revealed that extra musical training over only 15 months in early childhood leads to structural brain changes that diverge from typical brain development. Hyde et al. (2009) state in their study, “It is not possible from these findings to completely rule out that musicians may be born with preexisting biological predictors of musicality or that some children may have a certain genetically determined trajectory of cerebral development that may lead them to more likely continue to practice music relative to other children without this same predisposition. However, our findings do support the view that brain differences seen in adult musicians relative to non-musicians are more likely to be the product of intensive music training” (Hyde et al., 2009, p. 3022)
Question: Does music seem to run in your family? If so, do you think environment is a stronger determining factor or do you suspect musical ability is due to genetic disposition? Can you provide articles that support your point?
[/toggle]
[toggle title=”What Parts Brain Get a Workout When I’m Playing Music?”]
Many parts of the brain get a workout when playing music, including those that are shown to play a role in:
- Speech and language processing;
- Verbal areas;
- Visual spatial memory;
- Mathematical reasoning;
- Working memory;
- Metacognition (i.e. knowing about knowing or thinking about thinking); and,
- Motor abilities and tactile acuity (Roden et al., 2014).
When reading this section don’t let the terminology scare you off, the terms may seem intimidating, but I have included a short description of what the primary function of each brain area mentioned is. In the results of Groussard et al. (2014) section I have also included percentages of brain development size increases to illustrate the dramatic effect 18 months of music instruction has on the brain.
Evidence of increased brain plasticity in in musicians at many levels of development has also been found. Studies have shown the corpus callosum is larger in musicians than non-musicians (Schlaug, 2001). Pronounced increases in the inferior frontal gyrus of musicians (the area of the brain sometimes associated with response inhibition) has been observed, (Gaser & Schlaug, 2003; Schlaug, Jancke, Huang, & Steinmetz, 1995). However, some studies have determined that the function of the inferior frontal gyrus requires further study (Hampshire, Chamberlain, Monti, Duncan, & Owen, 2010). Increased grey and white matter throughout the brain could be linked to the multiple motor and sensory processes, planning, problem solving, and thinking that is required when striving for excellence in music (Gaser & Schlaug, 2003; Schlaug, 2006). Recent studies into increased auditory and language processing function in musicians have also shown that, “musicians have a refined hierarchy of internalized representations for auditory objects at both pre-attentive and attentive levels that supplies more faithful phonemic templates to decision mechanisms governing linguistic operations” (Bidelman, Weiss, Moreno, & Alain, 2014, p. 2662). A related study conducted by Roden et al. (2014) on attention, processing speed and auditory skills may support these findings. The study found that as little 18 months of extra music instruction increases visual attention, processing speed, and cognitive musical abilities when compared extra instruction recieved in the natural sciences. Additional studies have shown that a longer exposure to musical activity, even at the amateur level can have an effect in the:
- Left hippocampus (+ 17.8% episodic memory region);
- Left posterior cingulate gyrus (+23.5% integration of visual and emotional content);
- Left superior temporal cortex (+50% ability to decode and memorize music);
- Right insular (+14.6% emotional processing);
- Right middle and superior frontal cortices (+25% event synchronization); and,
- Right supplementary motor area (+26% pitch and timing after increase in this area shown after 15 years or more of experience)(Groussard et al., 2014).
Musical activity requires decoding and organizing auditory information which could account for some of the results of increased abilities. Processing speed and visual attention might be attributed to the temporal nature of music. Unlike some other visual processes (like reading a book) reading music requires the brain to keep track of temporal events while simultaneously navigating the visual landscape of sheet music or a score.
Processing speed throughout the brain is mostly attributed to increased myelin cell production. There are many types of myelin cells that are globally referred to as ‘white matter’. White matter connects the grey matter sections of the brain and allows for faster transfer of information between axons. Neurodegenerative disorders ranging from schizophrenia to Alzheimer’s Disease have been linked to changes in white matter volume and density (Bartzokis, 2011). Perhaps musical activities will be widely used as a preventative measure against such diseases in the future (Hyde, 2009).
Question: Are there other parts of the brain that you suspect might be affected by engaging in musical activities? Do you believe the claims made in the studies above? Please provide peer-reviewed support for your view.
[/toggle]
[toggle title=”How Can We Be Sure About What is Really Happening Inside the Brain?”]
When discussing brain function we must realize that many portions of the brain are active when performing directed activities such as performing music. Attempting to assign responsibility solely to one section of the brain can result in misleading findings. For that reason it’s important to remember that when studying brain function the structures of the brain work together, they are not independent from one another. We can equate this to how a car engine works; without question the spark plugs are key to the functioning of the internal combustion engine, but the spark plug is not the only part of the car responsible for the ignition of fuel, a variety of engine parts contribute to this process. Furthermore, the non-invasive nature in which studies are undertaken may also lead to contradictory findings that are susceptible to human error or oversight. Fortunately, the nature of science allows for mistakes, as it is driven by the correction and elimination of existing findings.
Question: Can you find articles that refute the claims made in the mentioned articles?
[/toggle]
[toggle title=”Will Information About Brain Function Help Me Play or Teach Music Better?”]
As musicians, music educators,music students and educational researchers we can actually use neuroimaging studies as a guide to improving our practices. The nature of collecting quantitative neurological data can be intoxicating, but it is always important to weigh the value of this data against other forms of research – not treat it as a means of blind justification. In addition, adhering to ethical standards and practices when young students are the primary participants can prove especially challenging and may have the potential to taint results. For example, Roden et al. (2014) excluded 100 students that pursued extra-curricular private lessons over and above the 45 minutes of extra instruction that was used as a variable in the study.
Students that exhibit a high interest in music may possess a genetic disposition towards plasticity in the areas of the brain that are most affected by musical activities (Hyde, 2009). Roden et al. (2014) did not identify the exclusion of 100 students that showed the highest interest in pursuing music activity as a limitation of the experiment. Their experiment may have yielded different results had the researchers had employed experimental techniques from the field of educational research such as a design experiment(Cobb, Confrey, Lehrer, & Schauble, 2003). In this case, and potentially many others, the accepted practices of neuroscience research may have garnered more refined results by incorporating accepted practices from other disciplines. The combination of neuroscience, cognitive psychology and educational research is increasingly being referred to as as ‘educational neuroscience’ (Hruby, 2012). Educational neuroscience used for music can aid music education and direct music pedagogy. It will also provide those responsible for writing and implementing educational policy with evidence showing how musical instruction benefits not only musicians but anyone who engages in musical activities.
Playing music has proven to be a unifying factor in our evolution and will continue to benefit our species.
[/toggle]
[toggle title=”References”]
Bartzokis, G. (2011). Alzheimer’s disease as homeostatic responses to age-related myelin breakdown. Neurobiology of Aging, 32(8), 1341-1371. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.08.007
Berlucchi, G., & Buchtel, H. A. (2009). Neuronal plasticity: historical roots and evolution of meaning. Experimental Brain Research, 192(3), 307-319. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1611-6
Bidelman, G. M., Weiss, M. W., Moreno, S., & Alain, C. (2014). Coordinated plasticity in brainstem and auditory cortex contributes to enhanced categorical speech perception in musicians. European Journal of Neuroscience, 40(4), 2662-2673. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ejn.12627
Brain training [Online image]. (2015).Retrieved April 20, 2015 from https://openclipart.org/detail/214552/brain-training
Cobb, P., Confrey, J., Lehrer, R., & Schauble, L. (2003). Design experiments in educational research. Educational researcher, 32(1), 9-13. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0013189X032001009
Diagram of neuron [Online image]. (2015).Retrieved April 20, 2015 from http://simple.wikipedia.org/wiki/Neuron
Gaser, C., & Schlaug, G. (2003). Brain structures differ between musicians and non-musicians. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23(27), 9240-9245. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(01)92488-7
Groussard, M., Viader, F., Landeau, B., Desgranges, B., Eustache, F., & Platel, H. (2014). The effects of musical practice on structural plasticity: The dynamics of grey matter changes. Brain and cognition, 90, 174-180. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2014.06.013
Hampshire, A., Chamberlain, S. R., Monti, M. M., Duncan, J., & Owen, A. M. (2010). The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: inhibition and attentional control. NeuroImage, 50(3), 1313-1319. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.109
Hruby, G. G. (2012). Three requirements for justifying an educational neuroscience. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(1), 1-23. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8279.2012.02068.x
Hyde, K. L., Lerch, J., Norton, A., Forgeard, M., Winner, E., Evans, A. C., & Schlaug, G. (2009). Musical training shapes structural brain development. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(10), 3019-3025. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5118-08.2009
Laufs, H. (2012). A personalized history of EEG–fMRI integration. NeuroImage, 62(2), 1056-1067. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.039
Neuronal synapse [Online image]. (2015).Retrieved April 20, 2015 from https://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Neuronal_Synapse.jpg
Roden, I., Könen, T., Bongard, S., Frankenberg, E., Friedrich, E. K., & Kreutz, G. (2014). Effects of music training on attention, processing speed and cognitive music abilities—findings from a longitudinal study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28(4), 545-557. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.3034
Schlaug, G. (2001). The Brain of Musicians. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 930(1), 281-299. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05739.x
Schlaug, G. (2006). Brain structures of musicians: executive functions and morphological implications. Music, motor control and the brain, 141-152. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199298723.003.0009
Schlaug, G., Jancke, L., Huang, Y., & Steinmetz, H. (1995). In vivo evidence of structural brain asymmetry in musicians. Science, 267(5198), 699-701. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.7839149
[/toggle]
[/accordion]
whoah this weblog is wonderful i really like reading your
posts. Keep up the great work! You understand, many people are looking around for this information, you could help them greatly.
Strangely enough I got this in an email this morning…
http://www.jneurosci.org/content/23/27/9240.full.pdf+html
Great observations Richard!
While I was researching this topic I ran into many papers that show quite a bit of engagement in multiple areas of the brain, even in the areas that are often associated with visual function. When pursuing this subject, I didn’t get into brain chemistry, I like your use of the word “gateway” perhaps you could say, Stevie Wonder is a gateway drug that may lead to a desire to play jazz. 🙂 If I run into anything on how listening to music affects brain structure I’ll pass it along to you.
Take it easy
Whar a fascinating field of research with so much potential to change the way we learn everything, not just music What effect would active listening to music have on brain development? It makes sense intuitively that it would not be as signiificant as playing music, but , when we listen to music, the brain is stimulated, oxytocin is produced, community is enhanced…. it must have some impact on development,
Also, if encouraging people to learn to play music is a social good, in that it helps brain development and health, can we “gateway” people to learn music by encouraging them to listen to music more?
This is a interesting blog. I will compare some personal life events to the theory.
1. Is musical ability inherited? Your great grandfather was a pianist, you have 2 uncles that played the trombone, and your father dabbled on the guitar. Your grandpa Craig was musical, but did not have the opportunity for lessons. He did play the harmonica and loved to sing. Your granny took piano lessons, but admits she didn’t have a strong aptitude for music.
2. You were given lessons at an early age and you had a natural ability to learn music. You had to work harder than your brother who picked up a guitar at age 3 and loved to practice guitar and piano. So I think Attitude was a big factor.
We had to set practice times with you and encourage you to practice. Once you went to Sask Summer School of the Arts, you started practicing on your own.
3 . I tried to learn the trumpet as an adult and had a hard time learning to read music, and playing the instrument. I wasn’t encouraged to continue, so didn’t . I really don’t have a desire to learn an instrument but love to listen to all types of music and sing along.
More tomorrow, for what my contribution is worth.
Mum
Thanks Mum :),
The amount of practice has been shown time and again to be the largest contributing factor to musical success. I know you have a library account that can access the articles and that you’d probably be interested in the one that points to practice time over genetics.
Gaser, C., & Schlaug, G. (2003). Brain structures differ between musicians and non-musicians. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23(27), 9240-9245. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(01)92488-7
Groussard, M., Viader, F., Landeau, B., Desgranges, B., Eustache, F., & Platel, H. (2014). The effects of musical practice on structural plasticity: The dynamics of grey matter changes. Brain and cognition, 90, 174-180. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2014.06.013
Hyde, K. L., Lerch, J., Norton, A., Forgeard, M., Winner, E., Evans, A. C., & Schlaug, G. (2009). Musical training shapes structural brain development. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(10), 3019-3025. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5118-08.2009
Roden, I., Könen, T., Bongard, S., Frankenberg, E., Friedrich, E. K., & Kreutz, G. (2014). Effects of music training on attention, processing speed and cognitive music abilities—findings from a longitudinal study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28(4), 545-557. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.3034
Schlaug, G. (2001). The Brain of Musicians. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 930(1), 281-299. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05739.x
Schlaug, G. (2006). Brain structures of musicians: executive functions and morphological implications. Music, motor control and the brain, 141-152. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199298723.003.0009
Schlaug, G., Jancke, L., Huang, Y., & Steinmetz, H. (1995). In vivo evidence of structural brain asymmetry in musicians. Science, 267(5198), 699-701. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.7839149
The reading is pretty heavy but the abstract says most of what you need to know. Roden et al. (2014) does mention that a genetic predisposition to music has not yet been ruled out, but it has also not yet been proven; definitely an area worth exploring.
Another factor that may have contributed to our own experiences could be the instruments that were chosen. Trumpet requires additional portions of the sensory and motor cortices that just using the hands like guitar does. Perhaps more grey and white matter development is needed between brain structures to maintain motivation. Here is a diagram that shows the distribution of responsibility for each body part in both of those areas. http://www.vgmuseum.com/mrp/multi/Essays/enemies-kev/homunculus2.jpg. Due to copyright reasons, I cannot post the image.
Thanks for posting
Did you come across any studies about the impact of Myelin Sheath damage (Multiple Sclerosis) on one’s musical ability?
Hey Jordana,
If you notice, Allison opted out of posting, I hope she will reconsider, I think she may have some interesting insights on the subject. In terms of how MS affects musical ability, I have not come across an article from that slant, BUT almost all of the neurological studies showed an increase in myelinated tissue. From what I can tell about why this tissue is formed, it seems the rhythmic nature of music has much to do with myelin production.
This article:
Bartzokis, G. (2011). Alzheimer’s disease as homeostatic responses to age-related myelin breakdown. Neurobiology of Aging, 32(8), 1341-1371. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.08.007
Is the closest one I have read that may provide an answer to your question, if you look at it, read the references and see something of interest to you, I can look for the article and pass along the link. I think logging into RPL may give you access to the articles. If not I extract the pertinent information for you.
Great questions thanks for engaging
Interesting article. I think that everybody naturally assumes that playing music requires specific parts of the brain – what I don’t think is as commonly thought about is that those brain structures rise up and grow to meet the challenge. And going by this article – not just a little bit either.
I would like to see a few things as a result of these studies which would be taking the same group and applying scholastic and intelligence testing to see if these appreciable gains actually turn into harnessable resources that the new and bettering musician can put into our activities with success – and not just in music.
The other study I would like to see is how far along in life can a person be, still take up music an accrue some of the benefits and how they pan out in other pursuits as well. They’ve proven that even octogenarians can respond positively to strength training by gaining increased muscle mass, tone and bone density. It would be interesting to know if there is a parallel here in music. If so, I think we may have found something that beats Lumuniosity.com hands down..
Hey Mark,
I’ll address your questions:
” taking the same group and applying scholastic and intelligence testing to see if these appreciable gains actually turn into harnessable resources that the new and bettering musician can put into our activities with success – and not just in music.”
I drew on many studies for this blog of varying ages and experiences as participants
Roden, I., Könen, T., Bongard, S., Frankenberg, E., Friedrich, E. K., & Kreutz, G. (2014). Effects of music training on attention, processing speed and cognitive music abilities—findings from a longitudinal study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28(4), 545-557. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.3034
This study used scholastic and intelligence testing as a baseline for their study, but did not address how these gains can be turned into harnessable resources as it was purely a neuroscience study .
you will probably be interested in one of the videos (Patel 2008) from the playlist when it comes to near and far-transfer effects of music
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=ZgKFeuzGEns&index=6&list=PLgkIZ87-dg0VWRf-8ur1aJvpdGkaw7qvn
The other question:
How far along in life can a person be, still take up music an accrue some of the benefits and how they pan out in other pursuits as well?
Hyde addresses the first part of this question, how far along in life can one accrue benefits, the answer is ANYTIME!! and we have the science to prove it from one of the articles I cited and many others.
Hyde, K. L., Lerch, J., Norton, A., Forgeard, M., Winner, E., Evans, A. C., & Schlaug, G. (2009). Musical training shapes structural brain development. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(10), 3019-3025. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5118-08.2009
The second part of the question is covered by:
Battro, A. M. (2010). The Teaching Brain. Mind, Brain, and Education, 4(1), 28-33. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-228X.2009.01080.x
and
Curtis, L., & Fallin, J. (2014). Neuroeducation and music: collaboration for student success. Music Educators Journal, 101(2), 52-56. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0027432114553637
These articles also touch on your comment about harnessable resources.
You may not be able to access these articles if you don’t have subscription, so I’m happy to answer or elabourate on any follow-up questions you may have.
Thanks Mark, I knew you’d have a few comments. As an aside, you first question may end up being a topic of my thesis or at very least a research paper.
Very cool. Thanks.
Next frontier carry out static & functional brain mapping (MRI/fMRI) as per an interdisciplinary comparison studies of all the arts (painting, dance, etc.) to learn what art form makes your brains grown in what areas and by how much.
Seems to me that if these benefits are transferable onto other pursuits (academic / personal) it makes a strong argument for restoring robust levels of arts funding in schools especially, along with more opportunities for society as a whole.
Hi Kelly
I may or may not be a good candidate. Given that I had brain surgery and other complications, may mean that I’m not a likely candidate. Let me know what you think.
Thanks and good luck
Allison
I think you are an ideal candidate because you underwent brain surgery and you are a musician
This just reinforces my desire to have my kids take music lessons. Increased grey matter is a good thing!
Thanks Jordana, Any questions about the blog?